Comparative Efficacy of Heuristic, Discovery-Based, and Manipulative-Aided Instruction on Student Achievement in Middle School Geometry
Keywords:
Geometry Education, Instructional Methods, Heuristic MethodAbstract
Background: Effective geometry instruction is fundamental to mathematical proficiency, yet students often struggle with its abstract concepts. While various student-centered pedagogical approaches have been proposed, there is a need for direct empirical comparison of their effectiveness. This study investigates the relative impact of three prominent instructional strategies: the heuristic method, discovery-based learning, and manipulative-aided instruction.
Purpose: The primary purpose of this study was to compare the efficacy of these three teaching methods against a traditional, lecture-based approach on sixth-grade students' achievement in geometry.
Methods: A quasi-experimental, pre-test/post-test control group design was employed with 124 sixth-grade students from four intact classes in a public middle school. Each class was randomly assigned to one of four conditions for an eight-week instructional period: (1) Heuristic Method, focusing on structured problem-solving; (2) Discovery Learning, emphasizing student-led exploration; (3) Manipulative-Aided Instruction, utilizing concrete physical objects; and (4) a Control Group receiving traditional instruction. A validated Geometry Achievement Test was administered before and after the intervention. Data were analyzed using Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA), with pre-test scores serving as the covariate.
Results: The ANCOVA revealed a statistically significant difference in post-test achievement scores among the four groups, F(3, 119) = 15.42, p < .001, partial η² = .28. Post-hoc comparisons using Tukey's HSD indicated that both the Manipulative-Aided (Adjusted M = 85.05) and Heuristic (Adjusted M = 81.85) groups were associated with significantly higher achievement than the Discovery Learning (Adjusted M = 74.58) and Traditional Control (Adjusted M = 73.90) groups. The manipulative group was associated with the highest mean score.
Conclusion: The findings suggest that instructional methods incorporating concrete experiences and structured problem-solving are associated with significantly more effective learning outcomes in geometry than either unguided discovery or traditional methods. The study provides strong evidence for integrating manipulatives and heuristic strategies into middle school mathematics curricula to enhance students' geometric understanding.
Downloads
References
Abonyi, O. S., & Umeh, V. O. (2014). Effects of heuristic method of teaching on students’ achievement in algebra. International Journal of Scientific & Engineering Research, 5(2), 1735-1740.
Alfieri, L., Brooks, P. J., Aldrich, N. J., & Tenebaum, H. R. (2011). Does Discovery-Based Instruction Enhance Learning? Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(1), 1-18. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021017
Baki, A., Kösa, T., & Güven, B. (2011). A comparative study of the effects of using dynamic geometry software and physical manipulatives on the spatial visualization skills of pre-service mathematics teachers. British Journal of Educational Technology, 42(2), 291–310. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8535.2009.01012.x
Baruiz, M., & Dioso, E. (2023). The use of manipulatives and its effects on the students’ mathematics achievement: An experimental study. EPRA International Journal of Environmental Economics, Commerce and Educational Management, 10(8), 55-65. https://doi.org/10.36713/epra14015.
Battista, M. T. (1999). The importance of spatial structuring in geometric reasoning. Teaching Children Mathematics, 6(3), 170-177. https://doi.org/10.5951/TCM.6.3.0170
Bower, C., Zimmermann, L., Verdine, B., Toub, T. S., Islam, S., Foster, L., ... & Golinkoff, R. M. (2020). Piecing together the role of a spatial assembly intervention in preschoolers’ spatial and mathematics learning: Influences of gesture, spatial language, and socioeconomic status. Developmental Psychology, 56(4), 686-698. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000899
Carbonneau, K., Marley, S. C., & Selig, J. P. (2012). A meta-analysis of the efficacy of teaching mathematics with concrete manipulatives. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(2), 380-400. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031084
Clements, D. H. (1999). 'Concrete' manipulatives, concrete ideas. Contemporary Issues in Early Childhood, 1(1), 45-60. https://doi.org/10.2304/ciec.2000.1.1.7
Clements, D. H., & Battista, M. T. (1992). Geometry and spatial reasoning. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning: A project of the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (pp. 420-464). Macmillan Publishing.
Damjanović, R. (2008). Konkretno iskustvo kao snažan oslonac u formiranju formalnog, apstraktnog mišljenja [Concrete experience as a strong foundation for the development of formal, abstract thinking]. Metodički obzori, 3(2), 35-45.
Diano Jr., F. M., Monterde, N. Y., & Diaz, J. M. (2021). Discovery approach in teaching mathematics among Grade Six Students. Solid State Technology, 64(2), 5157-5162.
Eby, J. W., Herrel, A. L., & Jordan, M. L. (2005). Teaching K-12 schools: A reflective action approach. Prentice Hall.
Gurung, R. K., & Chaudhary, D. K. (2022). Effectiveness of instruction with manipulative materials on fourth graders’ geometry learning achievement. Journal of Bhuwanishankar, 1(1), 69-84. https://doi.org/10.3126/jobs.v1i1.49495
Downloads
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
License
Copyright (c) 2025 Dr. Elena Vance, Prof. Samuel J. Carter

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
Authors retain the copyright of their manuscripts, and all Open Access articles are disseminated under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License 4.0 (CC-BY), which licenses unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided that the original work is appropriately cited. The use of general descriptive names, trade names, trademarks, and so forth in this publication, even if not specifically identified, does not imply that these names are not protected by the relevant laws and regulations.